The Transhuman Heresy
William Sims Bainbridge
George Mason University
Journal
of Evolution and Technology - Vol. 14 Issue
2 - August 2005 - pgs 1-10
http://jetpress.org/volume14/bainbridge.html
PDF Version
Abstract
Data from a pilot questionnaire begin the difficult process of
using social science methods to assess religious opposition to
human enhancement. Historical and theoretical considerations
suggest that the power of traditional religions is directly
threatened by Transhumanism, so the sacred monopolies can be
predicted to try to suppress it. The questionnaire data provide
initial support for this hypothesis, because highly religious
respondents show less favorable reactions to a variety of modes
of technological transcendence.
Buoyed by technological optimism and the hope that well-meaning
people can agree to disagree about the choices that each individual
must make, Transhumanists believe that we have reached the point in
history at which fundamental changes in our very natures have become
both possible and desirable. When humans can improve their own minds
and bodies technologically, then they will gain the intelligence and
longevity to devise even more methods for self-improvement. In a
positive feedback loop that vastly accelerates evolution, humans
could become like gods, and in so doing may put conventional
religion out of business. Thus it is in the vital interests of
Christianity and the other great world faiths to prevent human
technological transformation.
A Theoretical Framework
At the outset, it should be recognized that several different
theoretical perspectives exist that can frame the questions we ask
in empirical research. One of the more fully developed sociological
perspectives is sometimes called rational choice theory or
exchange theory
(Stark
and Bainbridge, 1980, 1985, 1987; Bainbridge, 2002a, 2003c).
All animals seek rewards and avoid costs – pleasure and pain – but
they differ in the sophistication of the metal apparatus with which
they do this. All mammals experience emotions like love and fear,
and the higher primates seem to have a sense of self-identity as
well. But only humans possess the complex minds necessary to frame
detailed plans, and the words with which to articulate ideas. In
pursuit of rewards and avoidance of costs, human seek
explanations with which to understand the contingencies and
algorithms that will guide behavior toward goals. Most recently,
the logical rigor of this theory has been explored through
artificial intelligence computer simulations that modeled the
emergence of religious beliefs among human beings who interacted
with each other in pursuit of valued rewards (Bainbridge, in press).
In the
absence of algorithms (plans, recipes, strategies) for attaining
desired rewards such as extended life or increased social power,
people will seek exchange partners who may be able to provide the
rewards. But until this point in human history, people could not
provide eternal life and some other valued rewards to each other.
Over time, people will exchange ideas and information about where
such rewards can be obtained, and rumor builds upon fantasy to
suggest there exists a supernatural realm where supernatural
exchange partners dwell, who might be willing to help humans deal
with their most desperate needs. At the same time, some humans
exploit this situation to attain status in their society as
religious specialists, priests whose role it is to mediate with the
supernatural beings.
Progressively, over generations in prehistoric time, magic and
religion arose in human culture as pseudosolutions to the problem of
providing help when people were obligated but unable to offer real
solutions. Gradually, local nature-religions arose, cultural fabrics
woven of innumerable promises decorated with symbols of presumably
powerful plants, animals, and forces of the environment. The rise of
agricultural civilization and invention of writing allowed many of
the primitive legends to be preserved and codified, a valuable
legacy for an emerging professional priesthood. Then religious
reformers began to appear – like Osiris, Moses, Buddha, Jesus, and
Mohammad – and soon after their deaths were transformed into
Messiahs or Messengers of God, whose mythologies served the
propaganda needs of progressively larger and better-organized sacred
bureaucracies.
A fully
developed, modern religious bureaucracy suppresses magical
movements, because they threaten the monopoly on supernaturalism
enjoyed by the established sacred authority, and it will resist real
solutions to human problems of powerlessness and mortality for the
same reason. The church is the last remaining absolute monarchy –
God, after all, is its Lord – and its loyal defenders will oppose
any attempt to establish a republic. Transhumanism, especially in
its democratic variants (Hughes, 2004), seeks to empower each
individual to become whatever he or she wishes. It has emerged at a
point in the history of science and technology when this grand goal
may soon become possible, as suggested by a series of major reports
concerning converging technologies for improving human performance
(Roco and Bainbridge, 2003; Roco and Montemagno, 2004; Bainbridge
and Roco, 2005). True human freedom would violate the divine right
of kings, or the rites of divine kings, so one might predict that
all but the most secularized forms of religion will brand it
sacrilegious.
A broad
theoretical perspective such as this can be refined or tested only
by an extensive research program, and any one study offers only
limited confirmation, disconfirmation, or elaboration. This article
reports results from one small part of such a program.
Religious Opposition to Human Transformation
An
initial reconnaissance of the relationship between religion and
technological transcendence was carried out with a pilot
questionnaire study of attitudes toward radical technological
possibilities that was distributed initially at the 2003 Transvision
conference, then through classes at religious colleges and a secular
university. The 435 respondents were by no means a random sample of
the population, but included a number of Transhumanists, others who
might be sympathetic to the idea of human enhancement, and religious
people who were recruited because they presumably have very
different beliefs and attitudes from those of Transhumanists. The
primary point was to administer the pilot questionnaire to people
with strong views who were thus likely to highlight any deficiencies
in the items so that they could be improved for a future study that
sought a broader and more representative sample. However, until such
time as we have more reliable data, the results of this pilot study
offer tentative but clear insights.
The
questionnaire began with seven brief stories about people who are
exploring some of the new technological possibilities for
transcendence, asking respondents to read each one and judge whether
the plans of the person in the story are good or bad. There were
actually eight versions of each story, varying three dichotomous
variables, and at random a given respondent received just one.
Here, the questionnaire was
prototyping material following a standard method for incorporating
experiments in questionnaires, called the vignette method (Alexander
and Becker, 1978; Sniderman and Grob, 1996).
For example, here are two versions of a story about cryonic
suspension:
Story
I, Version 1: Michael is a senior scientist in a medical research
laboratory who has an incurable disease. He has become interested in
a process called cryonic suspension. This involves carefully
freezing a person's body, perhaps for as long as several decades,
then thawing and reviving the person to continue his life. Michael
has decided to enter cryonic suspension himself, so that his disease
can be cured by the medical science of the future.
Story I,
Version 2: Mary is a high school health
teacher who is perfectly healthy. She has become interested in a
process called cryonic suspension. This involves carefully freezing
a person's body, perhaps for as long as several decades, then
thawing and reviving the person to continue her life. Mary has
decided to enter cryonic suspension herself, so that she can see
what the future will be like.
For
each of the seven stories, half of the respondents read a story
about a man, and half read a story about a woman. There were two
reason why gender was varied in this way. First, if we asked just
about one gender, then we would not know if the results generalized
to the other. Second, research supports the common stereotype that
males are more likely to take physical risks than females (Flynn
et al., 1994), so by varying gender at random we can explore
whether this applies to futuristic risks like cryonic suspension.
The stories also varied in whether the person was an expert in the
field, and thus able to make an informed decision – here whether the
person was a senior medical research scientist or a high school
health teacher. And a third variable differed across stories, but
often gave the person a special motivation for taking the risk, such
as having a currently incurable disease.
In
fact, the different experimental manipulations appeared to have very
little impact on respondents' judgments of the stories. About 21
percent of people who read the story about Michael think cryonic
suspension would be a good idea, compared with 18 for those who read
about Mary. This is a small difference, and could easily be due to
chance. Indeed, averaging across all seven stories to get more
stable statistics, respondents thought the ideas were good for a
male protagonist 31 percent of the time, and 33 percent if the
protagonist were female. The difference between expert protagonists
and non-experts is coincidentally about the same, 31 versus 33.
Although many published vignette studies have reported significant
differences, it is not uncommon for the experimental manipulation to
prove far weaker than other variables, such as the personal
characteristics of respondents (e.g. McKinlay et al., 2002).
This is
important for the present analysis, because it suggests that the
ideology of the respondent could overpower any specific details of
the concrete situation described in a story, in determining
responses. If the gender and expertise of the protagonist in the
story do not matter to respondents, then we may wonder whether the
respondents are willing to grant protagonists the right to decide
their own fate. After briefly explaining the content of the other
stories, we will see whether respondents' religious views are a more
powerful determinant of reactions.
The
second story turned out not to be controversial enough, because so
many respondents thought the person had a good plan that differences
in opinion cannot be correlated with other variables. It concerned
using advanced multi-media computer technology to record all of
one's experiences, which could be a step toward cybernetic
immortality but was described blandly enough that it did not provoke
negative reactions. The third story stimulated more disagreement,
because it took that idea to an extreme conclusion:
Story
III, Version 1: Albert is a very successful brain surgeon. He reads
a lot about both computers and the human brain, and he believes it
is possible to transfer a person's mind from the brain to a
computer. Recently he has become involved in a research project to
accomplish this, by having his own mind scanned in, using a process
that will destroy his brain.
The fourth story drew moderate reactions, and describes a method of
personality capture (Bainbridge, 2003a) somewhat between stories two
and three in terms of how radical the technological idea is:
Story
IV, Version 1: Elizabeth is the head of a computer science
laboratory. She has become interested in the concept of uploading a
human personality to a computer, using a variety of psychological
tests and opinion surveys. She has assembled a huge computerized
collection of questionnaires and has already answered forty thousand
questions. She hopes that progress in information technology and
artificial intelligence will allow her ideas to influence people
even after her death.
The fifth story concerned human reproductive cloning. An article
published previously in The Journal of Evolution and Technology
(Bainbridge, 2003b) and based on a different dataset found that
religious respondents were especially hostile to the idea of
reproductive cloning. Story number six was about use of
nanotechnology robots (nanites)
to
clean and repair the circulatory system, and it has been analyzed in
an article published in The Journal of Nanoparticle Research
(Bainbridge, 2004). The final story builds on the earlier items and
explores an idea presented in a book published by NASA (Bainbridge,
2002b):
Story
VII, Version 1: Carl is an electrician working for an airline who
has always dreamed of traveling through space to another star. He
has joined a group that believes it is possible to record an
individual's personality and genetic code, send them to a distant
planet, and reconstitute the person there to begin a new,
extraterrestrial life. He is very excited about the most recent
advances in the space program, and has volunteered to have himself
recorded for launch on a future interstellar mission.
Religious Beliefs and Attitudes toward Technological Transcendence
The seven stories do not directly attack religion, but they have two
qualities that might offend religious sensitivities. First, they
suggest that it may be possible to improve human nature to a
significant degree, thus implicitly faulting God's handiwork in
creating humans. Second, they hint that it may be possible to
achieve technological immortality, thus stealing one of God's
supposed prerogatives.
One of the miscellaneous items in the survey gives us a measure of
the intensity of the respondent's religious faith. It asked the
person to select one of the following choices: "I
don't believe in God." "I don't know whether there is a God and I
don't believe there is any way to find out." "I don't believe in a
personal God, but I do believe in a Higher Power of some kind." "I
find myself believing in God some of the times, but not at others."
"While I have doubts, I feel that I do believe in God." "I know God
really exists and I have no doubts about it." This is a widely used
religious belief item that has proven itself in many prior studies.
Given that we intentionally oversampled religious conservatives (in
the relatively religious United States), fully 56.2 percent selected
the final option, and in Table 1 we compare them with the 43.8
percent who selected any of the other options.
Table 1: Percent Saying the Idea is Good by Belief in God
|
Saying the Thing is Good |
No Doubt God Exists |
Doubt about God's Existence |
Cryonic suspension |
13% |
28% |
Recording all one's experiences |
77% |
81% |
Having one's mind scanned in |
10% |
28% |
Uploading a human personality |
22% |
34% |
Cloning oneself |
5% |
19% |
Nanites inserted into blood stream |
46% |
57% |
Send personality to distant planet |
11% |
27% |
|
|
|
Average of 7 stories |
26% |
39% |
Among people who selected one of the five choices indicating some
doubt in the existence of God, 28 percent felt that cryonic
suspension was a good idea, compared with only 13 percent among
those having complete faith in God. Discounting the second story,
which was too bland to elicit strong opinions, we see a solid
tendency for religiously faithful people to reject the other
transcendent technologies as well. On average, only 26 percent of
highly religious respondents think the plan in a story is good,
compared with 39 percent of those with a range of religious views.
In a larger study, with sufficient numbers of respondents to compare
across all different beliefs in God, we would expect to find an even
great range of reactions to the stories.
Table 2
shows results comparable to those in Table 1 for a second religion
item that measures a very different attitudinal dimension.
"Organized religion" was one of a set of four "institutions in this
country," the other three being medicine, the scientific community,
and major companies. The instructions asked: "As far as the people
running these institutions are concerned, would you say you have a
great deal of confidence, only some confidence, or hardly any
confidence at all in them?" About 25 percent of the respondents
expressed a great deal of confidence in organized religion, whereas
36 percent had some confidence and 37 percent had hardly any. For
the scientific community, the comparable percentages were 30, 52,
and 14. The table shows that approval of technological
transcendence is negatively associated with confidence in religion,
and positively associated with confidence in science.
Table 2: Percent Saying the Idea is Good by Confidence in Religion
and Science
|
Organized Religion |
Scientific Community |
A great deal |
Only some |
Hardly any |
A great deal |
Only some |
Hardly any |
Cryonic suspension |
14% |
16% |
25% |
33% |
15% |
9% |
Recording all one's experiences |
81% |
80% |
78% |
85% |
76% |
72% |
Having one's mind scanned in |
9% |
18% |
25% |
27% |
14% |
16% |
Uploading a human personality |
21% |
29% |
33% |
39% |
24% |
19% |
Cloning oneself |
4% |
11% |
18% |
16% |
10% |
9% |
Nanites inserted into blood stream |
44% |
51% |
57% |
63% |
49% |
40% |
Send personality to distant planet |
10% |
16% |
27% |
30% |
52% |
12% |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Average of 7 stories |
26% |
32% |
38% |
42% |
34% |
25% |
Comments from Religious Respondents
After each of the seven stories, the respondent was encouraged to
write comments, and many availed themselves of this opportunity. The
religious objections to reproductive cloning were reported in an
earlier article that was based on a different dataset (Bainbridge,
2003), so here we will only briefly examine criticisms of other
technologies that reveal how pious people think about such issues.
Some religious respondents felt that the technology violates God's
plan for us. That would be bad not only because it defies the Lord
–
as they see it
–
but also because they believe all human life gains its meaning from
God's plan. "Cryonic freezing is something I disagree with. God put
us on this earth at a certain time for a certain reason." On the
same topic, others wrote, "We as humans do not have the right to
play God in such a manner;" "I don't believe in altering God's
plan;" "You are messing with God's work and his plan for you." "This
is wrong, to continue life like this is against everything I believe
in. When God gives you a time to die it is your time. This is acting
too God like in my opinion." The same objection was leveled against
interstellar colonization: "Other planets are not made for humans to
live on. If God wanted us to live on those planets he would have put
us there.
On brain scanning, one worried, "I don't see that happening, and if
it does that will be very scaring. Will we be doing God's work?"
Another complained, "It is wrong to play God with mind, body, or
spirit." About computerized personality capture through
question-answering, one said, "From a religious point of view, I see
this as 'playing God.' I feel that God ultimately decides how long
one is here and I'm relatively certain that the development of such
technology (God willing) would head to the demise of all humanity."
Another asserted, "It is corrupt to think someone wants their human
personality to be alive after they die. You should just leave all
that stuff alone and let God do his work."
Several of the stories suggest not merely enhancement but
technological immortality, and religious respondents think the
problem of human death has already been solved, but by God, not by
human inventiveness.
On recording all one's experiences, one religious respondent wrote,
"For educational purposes about his life it is ok, but to achieve
immortality? That is only done by accepting Jesus." In response to
two of the personality capture items, another commented, "Only God
gives eternal life through His son Jesus... There is no way to
become immortal in this world, unless you believe in a loving God
who sent His son to die for you." A third respondent offered a more
complete analysis: "While recording and documenting one's personal
life experiences is something I find realistic and even interesting,
I don't see the connection between that and achieving a kind of
immortality. While we analyze and often relive our memories, the
things we go through are merely experiences through which our
characters and faith in God are enhanced. If we were meant to be
immortal on earth, God would've created us so."
A religious conception implies a very different way of thinking
about human personality, that may render technological solutions
implausible to the religious person. One said, "Immortality is a
matter of the spirit, not the physical.
The
mind is more than a collection of electrical impulses and
neurotransmitters or neurons. It's linked to the spirit, which isn't
transferable to a database." Another thought cryonic
suspension would fail: "If
one believes that a human being is made up of both soul and body,
then no matter what is done to the body, if a man's soul departs,
then he is dead. There's nothing wrong with wanting to have a long
life, but when a man's time is up, it's up and no amount of
scientific advancement can prevent the soul from leaving the body."
"A person's being is not just his brain, but his soul.
Unfortunately, the soul needs the brain to exist on Earth." "I think
the human soul and mind is something that could never be contained
in any computer, no matter how sophisticated or advanced." "You may
be able to transfer one's personality to a computer, but not their
soul, and that is what counts."
Some
respondents explained that a soul or spirit is what gives us many
fundamental human characteristics. "Her ideas may remain in the
computer, but her soul is in her body. Even if she lives on
mentally, she will be dead and it won't be here, just her thoughts
(no feelings or emotions)." "The mind is not solely a collection of
ones and zeros processing data and spewing out results. That is one
function, but there is also the spirit of a man that sustains him.
Without a man's spirit the mind would be a mere 'super computer'
without life, free-will and choice."
Belief in the soul poses a difficulty as well for interstellar
travel in the form of an information pattern: "Duplicate her? So
there's a copy on another planet? Don't think so. Nor would it
really be her. Again the missing ingredient is the spirit that God
gives to a person and they develop, some more, some less. That's not
copyable. If this means 'de-constitute' Carol to reconstitute her,
again it doesn't work. It's like suicide. If physically possible,
her mind would be there, and the mind is an integral part of the
soul of a person!" "A person's soul cannot be reconstituted, so this
plan of reconstituting a person –
even if they have their personality and genetic code
– is flawed. 'The body without the
spirit is dead.'"
Finally, to some respondents pursuing these technologies for
personal gain would not serve appropriate spiritual goals. "It's
man's attempt to achieve the spiritual or eternal life on his own
without having to go through the proper channels.
– Probably so he can say 'I did it
myself without God' – which
basically is pride, and pride in my opinion is the opposite of true,
sacrificial love." "Why waste your life working towards a false
sense of eternity, when there is so much to cherish, learn and love
in the life we are given?"
Additional Measures
The pilot questionnaire also included 20 agree-disagree statement
items about science and technology, including four that express
views Transhumanists might hold:
"Humanity is on the verge of evolving into a higher form of life."
"Technological convergence – combining nanotechnology,
biotechnology, information technology and cognitive science – will
greatly improve human abilities." "Cryonics (freezing a person's
body until medical science is able to cure its diseases) will enable
people to survive otherwise fatal accidents and illnesses."
"Research on human cloning should be encouraged, because it will
greatly benefit science and medicine." Table 3 reports data on these
four statements in favor of technological transcendence, plus one
statement opposed: "There should be a law against cloning human beings." Five
responses were offered: Strongly agree, Agree, Neutral, Disagree,
and Strongly disagree. Table 3, combines the two positive responses,
Agree, and Strongly agree.
Table
3: Religion and Agreement with Statements about Technological
Transcendence
|
Agree with Statement |
Confidence in Organized Religion |
|
No Doubt God Exists |
Doubt about God's Existence |
A great deal |
Only some |
Hardly any |
In favor of technological transcendence: |
Humanity is on the verge of evolving into a higher form
of life. |
19% |
23% |
16% |
20% |
26% |
|
Technological convergence – combining nanotechnology,
biotechnology, information technology and cognitive
science – will greatly improve human abilities. |
40% |
59% |
40% |
51% |
53% |
|
Cryonics (freezing a person's body until medical science
is able to cure its diseases) will enable people to
survive otherwise fatal accidents and illnesses. |
17% |
31% |
17% |
21% |
31% |
|
Research on human cloning should be encouraged, because
it will greatly benefit science and medicine. |
10% |
38% |
10% |
20% |
33% |
|
Opposed to technological transcendence: |
There should be a law against cloning human beings. |
81% |
38% |
84% |
63% |
46% |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Highly
religious people are most rejecting of cloning, but there is also a
difference in the direction predicted on the other items. This is
further evidence that supernatural transcendence competes with
technological transcendence.
Conclusion
This
pilot study indicates that especially religious people may indeed be
substantially more likely than other people to reject various forms
of technological transcendence. The results suggest that a more
extensive study could be worthwhile, employing a larger number of
respondents. Ideally, one would want to use a random sample of the
population. However, given the great cost of large random samples,
one might consider a widely-distributed Web-based questionnaire
including a number of calibration items that could be used to weight
results to approximate a random sample.
Before
such a study is done, it will be important to identify measures of a
number of other concepts that should be included in the
questionnaire, to achieve maximum scientific payoff. Other religion
measures should cover the major dimensions of belief, practice, and
affiliation. Similarly, other measures of attitudes toward
technological transcendence and transhumanism should be included.
Finally, such a study should explore other factors that might shape
attitudes, independently of religion, such as educational level,
general orientation toward science and technology, and personality.
One would predict that religion is not the only factor determining
attitudes toward technological transcendence, although it probably
is a powerful one.
References
Alexander, Cheryl S., and Henry Jay Becker. 1978. "The Use of
Vignettes in Survey Research," Public Opinion Quarterly
42(1): 93-104.
Bainbridge, William Sims. 1987. "Science
and Religion: The Case of Scientology," pp. 59-79 in The Future
of New Religious Movements, edited by David G. Bromley and
Phillip E. Hammond. Macon, Georgia: Mercer University Press.
_____. 2002a "A Prophet's Reward: Dynamics of Religious
Exchange," pp. 63-89 in Sacred Markets, Sacred Canopies,
edited by Ted G. Jelen. Lanham, Maryland: Rowman and Littlefield.
_____. 2002b. "The Spaceflight Revolution
Revisited," pp. 39-64 in Looking Backward, Looking Forward,
edited by Stephen J. Garber. Washington, D.C.: National Aeronautics
and Space Administration.
_____. 2003a. "Massive Questionnaires for
Personality Capture," Social Science Computer Review 21 (3):
267-280.
_____. 2003b. "Religious Opposition to Cloning,"
Journal of Evolution and Technology, 13, retrieved
September 1, 2004 from www.jetpress.org/volume 13/bainbridge.html.
_____. 2003c. "Sacred Algorithms: Exchange Theory of
Religious Claims," in Defining Religion edited by Arthur L.
Greil and David G. Bromley. Oxford: JAI/Elsevier (Volume 10 of
Religion and the Social Order).
_____. 2004. "Religion and Science," Futures
36: 1009-1023.
_____.
In press. God From the Machine:
Artificial Intelligence Models of Religious Cognition. Walnut
Grove, California: AltaMira.
Bainbridge, William Sims, and Mihail C. Roco (eds.). 2005.
Converging Technologies for Human Progress. Berlin: Springer.
Flynn, J., P. Slovic, and C. K. Mertz. 1994. "Gender, Race, and
Perception of Environmental Health Risks," Risk Analysis
14(6): 1101-1108.
Hughes, James J. 2004. Citizen Cyborg: Why Democratic Societies
Must Respond to the Redesigned Human of the Future. Cambridge,
Massachusetts: Westview Press.
McKinlay, John B., Ting Lin, Karen Freund, and Mark Moskowitz. 2002.
"The Unexpected Influence of Physician Attributes on Clinical
Decisions: Results of an Experiment," Journal of Health and
Social Behavior 43: 92-106.
Roco, Mihail C., and William Sims Bainbridge (eds.). 2003.
Converging Technologies for
Improving Human Performance. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer.
Roco, Mihail C., and Carlo D. Montemagno (eds.). 2004. The Coevolution
of Human Potential and Converging Technologies. New York: New
York Academy of Sciences (Annals of the New York Academy of
Sciences, volume 1013).
Sniderman, Paul M., and Douglas B. Grob. 1996. "Innovations in
Experimental Design in Attitude Surveys," Annual Review of
Sociology 22: 377-399.
Stark, Rodney, and William Sims Bainbridge. 1980. "Towards a Theory
of Religion: Religious Commitment," Journal for the Scientific
Study of Religion 19:114-128.
Stark,
Rodney and William Sims Bainbridge. 1985. The Future of Religion.
Berkeley: University of California Press.
Stark,
Rodney and William Sims Bainbridge. 1987. A Theory of Religion.
New York: Toronto/Lang.
|